STIRLING STUDIES 1: S. M. STIRLING AND THE AGGLUTINATIVE LANGUAGES

 

B. Lukács

 

President of the Matter Evolution Subcommittee of the Geonomy Scientific Committee of HAS

 

CRIP RMKI, H-1525 Bp. 114, Pf. 49, Budapest, Hungary

 

lukacs@rmki.kfki.hu

 

ABSTRACT

            S. M. Stirling is writing books in Alternate History. Besides the obvious evolutionary connections, these books suggest lots of interesting scientific or scholarly problems, some discussed in the books, some not. Here I discuss one such recurrent in his Nantucket series: are indeed agglutinative languages almost impossible to be learnt by IE speakers?

 

0. PROLOGUE

            S. M. Stirling is producing a big variety of Alternative History sci-fi’s. Alternative History is a subclass of sci-fi’s, where the fantasy does not enjoy its unlimited freedom. In an ideal AH story only one event has other outcome than on our timeline (that is the PoD, Point of Divergence), especially an event which "might have happened as well otherwise", by other words, where even now we do not know if the particular outcome was necessary or not. (Here I omit philosophical discussions of Free Will, determinism/indeterminism, the Everett Theory of Quantum Mechanical Measurement and such, for simplicity.) So at PoD one event has another outcome, and then the author continues the story, with as strict "causality" as possible. Of course the PoD acts then as an initial condition different than in Our TimeLine (OTL). Then the author elaborates this Alternative History.

            Now, we do not yet know if such concurrent timelines can be realised "synchronously", or not. (Against the a priori impossibility of the alternative see Everett's famous paper [1].) But even this were impossible, the elaboration is edifying. Anyway, if one believes in Free Will (this is religion-dependent, of course), one should think over the alternatives before any important decision.

            The machinery can be demonstrated by means of, e.g., Poul Anderson's works. In Eutopia [2] a modern (c. 1970 AD) Greek social researcher from a timeline where Alexander III of Macedon did not drink himself exactly into death but after some time recovered, so Aristotle did not have to leave the Lyceum  in 322 AD, can switch timelines, and explores one where Alexander did die prematurely, but the Tours-Poitiers battle in 732 AD had an outcome opposite than in OTL. So the Frankish Kingdom became weaker, the Pippin-Charlemagne line could not substitute the Merovings, so in the next 200 years Dane Vikings & Magyars destroyed the Western Christian civilisation. In 1970 AD Danes can be found in Canada & Minnesota (Anderson's state) and Magyars along the Mississippi (Dakoty). In another mininovel [3] Sulmanu-Assaridu King of Assyria took Jerusalem in 701 BC (this was the probable outcome; we still do not know what happened, but the Holy Bible is also not too definite). Then no Judaism from VII c. BC upward, so no Christianism either; the Roman Empire breaks down in a somewhat less dramatic way, in 1976 AD the Visigothic Kingdom is the strongest Mediterranean power, Mithraism is the strongest religion on Southwest, Mazdaism in Southeast, and the North is pagan. And again: Danes in Canada & Minnesota and Magyars in the Mississippi Basin.

            S. M. Stirling works in the AH scheme. In one sequence (Lords of Creation [4], [5]) the PoD was 200 My in the past; a superhuman civilisation terraformed Venus & Mars, and from time to time they imported new animals & plants to the other two planets. Terrestrial history was practically not influenced until 1947 AD, when astronomers detected the habitable planetary surfaces. Therefore Cold War went to space. The Draka sequence of Stirling puts the PoD to somewhere at the second half of the XVIIIth century, Northern American loyalists go to South Africa, so the XXth century will be very, very different. (But this sequence is par excellence Military SF, so I ignore it completely.)

            Now, Stirling & Flint invented a somewhat different new AH scenario. (I think Stirling was somewhat earlier, but Flint in the 1632 Universe of Assiti Shards has a big net of collaboration, so novels are published rather fast.)

            The idea is "simple". A tremendously superhuman ET civilisation sends back a negligible part of our contemporary Earth into the past, and replaces the surface with the past one. In Flint's 1632 [6] a West Virginian small miner town of 2000 is sent back to Thuringy in 1631, the heyday of the Thirty Year War.

            Stirling's somewhat similar scheme produced so far 6 full novels, 1 novelette, a 7th novel is just being published, and 1 in preparation [7-15]. In [7-10] Something sends back the Island of Nantucket and its immediate maritime neighbourhood from 1998 AD to 1250 BC. The Nantucketers sometimes discuss what may have happened, and they agree that most probably the 1250 BC Nantucket emerged in 1998 AD, but, of course, they cannot check. Then [11] is the opposite viewpoint: post-Change USA. Some Superhuman agency slightly changed some Physical Laws on the 1998 surface of Earth, so that the Boyle-Mariotte Gas Law changes, so gasoline engines do not work, gunpowder merrily burns but does not detonate, steam engines can work only with negligible efficiency and there is no electric current (I do not yet understand the relation of the last change to the previous ones). High energy 1998 civilisation breaks down and the overwhelming majority of Humanity dies out; but at some centers good organisers found a post-Change civilisation, a viable hybrid of XXth century and High Middle Ages. And then [14] starts to sew together [7-10] with [11-13], and with more Realities as well. I conjecture that in a few years Stirling will write something establishing a link between the Lords of Creation and the Nantucket anomaly.

            While this opus is not yet comparable in size and details with that of Honoré de Balzac, it may be later; and while Balzac had to know only his present society and its immediate past, Stirling is confronted with scientific problems, and with a number of different societies. True, the scientific problems are not direct. Earthpeople do not understand the Lords of Creation; not even the bioscience of Martian hominids. But Stirling should; or at least has to make the impression he knows more about than us.

            So far he generally fulfils the expectations. But the opus is a challenge for somebody (I mean myself) who is the President of a Matter Economy Subcommittee. Evolution of sciences, evolution of languages, evolution of hominids...

            I am going to make a series of comments of that opus. That will not be criticism. Nobody is interested if I like or dislike a book. Rather I am going to discuss different items in the books from the viewpoint of the actual science (or, sometimes, scholarship).

            Sometimes this is really a challenge. E.g. Lords of Creation definitely use a physics unknown for us. And our present physics is not the Physics. For 2000 years it was not only elementary experience but also Fundamental Theory that there is no Motion (at least, except for transients,) without Force. People generally believe that Aristotle was stupid and the later generations did not dare innovate; but Aristotle did know that his theory was not yet complete. See his own words about the problem of ballistics (spears and arrows) [16]. In Chap. 32 of Mechanics he writes: "Why is it that an object which is thrown eventually comes to a standstill? Does it stop when the force which started it fails, or because the object is drawn in a contrary direction, or is it due to its downward tendency, which is stronger than the force which threw it? Or is it absurd to discuss such questions, while the principle escapes us?" (Italics are mine.) The paragraph is clear enough even it contains only questions. The Stagirite did know that ballistics was at best very difficult to describe even in his theory; and he was not able to do. Still, missiles flew; and later somebody would improve even his description.

            Before Einstein's success not only everybody in physics was convinced that Flow of Time was independent of spatial motions; everybody believed that even a doubt would be meaningless. Special Relativity appeared a mere 103 years. Similarly, until the formulation of Quantum Mechanics (more or less 1926) physicists were convinced that any object has its sharp momentum and position, although maybe we do not know them; and similarly that one object cannot be simultaneously here and there. It seemed defetism to question the first statement and unscientific magic the second. That was 82 years ago.

            Our present science will be so primitive and untrue for the XXVth century as the Aristotelian physics for us. Still, Aristotle was a true scientist (while Platon, Marcion, Plotinos and Melanchton were not), as a true physicist as  e.g. Galileo, Newton and Einstein. Our present physics will not be substituted by a free choice of personal theories (Aquarius or not Aquarius) but by a still unknown new theory explaining what the previous theory did and some further ones as well. And then, after a time the next theory will be disproven as well...

            Stirling's books show alternate realities. We know only one of them; but there may be others. But are the stories consistent at least with our present knowledge plus the assumption that Alternate Realities can exist? This is a nontrivial but very interesting question.

            In the first study I reflect to the opinion suggested by [7]-[9] (most explicitly in Chap. 7 of [7] and Chap. 26 of [8]) that agglutinative languages are hopeless to learn, at least for Indo-Europeans. I do not believe this; in the thousand years of the Regnum Hungariae many Germans, Slovakians, Rusyns, Roumanians and Serbs learnt agglutinative Magyar, the language of the central parts of the country. Not everybody spoke it, and some spoke not too stylishly; but multiplicity of nominal cases, more than one endings simultaneously attached to a root, Vowel Harmony and such were never insurmountable barriers. Indo-European languages as Lithuanian or Hittite are indeed somewhat simpler; but agglutinative languages are not absurdly complicated.

 

1. STONEHENGE’S AGGLUTINATIVE BUILDERS

            In [7] Nantucket, some 7500 humans on an island of negligible agricultural possibilities, with 1 ship of the Home Guard, is swept back to 1250 BC. They would starve; except if they send an expedition to Britain, where some Bronze Age agriculture exists, so wheat, barley, cows and horses can be bartered, for XXth century steel & glass. They expect proto-Celts in Britain; but their only historian speaks only Homeric Greek, plus a little Mycenian. But the astronomer who was able to make the chronology is half Lithuanian. Lithuanian is a very conservative Indo-European language, so maybe she will be able to understand something.

            Indeed, she is able to start some language course with a rescued castaway; although I simply cannot believe that an astronomer would try with Pl2 in "Do you understand?" as "Ar mane spurantate?" instead of Sg2. But the Nantucketers give up halfway the effort to work out understanding; instead they hire a Tartessian merchant, slaver &c. who speaks some Mycenian, good proto-Celt and a little of the other language of Britain, that of the builders of Stonehenge.

            The Tartessians are, of course, early ancestors of Basque, so agglutinative. And the aborigines of Merry Old Britain are also speakers of an agglutinative language. Agglutinative languages are rather hopeless (at least for Stirling) [7-9], so the Tartessian expert is unavoidable. Interestingly enough, they do not have a Magyar among the population of Nantucket!

            Now, in Chap. 2 I discuss the meaning of agglutinative, while in Chap. 3-5 I give a demonstration about noun, adjective, numeral and pronoun agglutination from the most agglutinative language of the Old World, Magyar, my first language. Sect. 6 is about an agglutinative speciality: postpositions. While they are analogous to prepositions of English, Latin, Russian &c., postpositions can turn into true Case endings, and they do this indeed in Magyar. Sect. 7 is the Conclusion, and the Appendix deals with Vowel Harmony Rules.

 

2. WHAT IS AN AGGLUTINATIVE LANGUAGE?

            Modern linguists do not like too much the term agglutinative; the overwhelming majority of them is not the speaker of Magyar. True, the definition, from a century in the past, was not quite strict. Still, it was useful; you may, e.g., follow [17]. If I try to discuss the problems about Swindapa Kurlelo's agglutinative language, then a classic work from the first half of last century is just adequate.

            I am not following exactly [17], but you can find every necessary definitions there.

            There are isolating, agglutinating, flectating and polysynthetic languages, with all the possible transitions as well.

            Isolating languages in the ideal case put together unchanged blocks ("words"). The par excellence isolating language is (Mandarin) Chinese; it can be written with pure ideograms since the blocks are unchanged. (Japanese uses the same ideograms, but not being isolating it needs syllabaric signs as well.) Practically English and Afrikaans are also isolating languages; they almost totally gave up the original grammatical endings. (And search engines are much less capable in non-isolating languages.)

            Flectating languages can be demonstrated by the older Indo-European languages. Latin is extinct and Italian lost the various endings of nouns. (The system kept very much of the verbal flectating forms, but I

ignore verbs in the whole study, for simplicity.) Practically the only thing you can do with an Italian noun is to form its plural: Italiano>Italiani, but Italiana>Italiane. However the old system is hardly simplified in the modern Slavic & Baltic languages. (For this reason is the half-Lithuanian astronomer included in [6-8].) The Common IE language had 8 (or 9?) different nominal cases both in Singular and in Plural (the system was rudimentary in Dual), and Lithuanian still keeps 7 as:

 

Nom

arklys

horse

Gen

arklio

of horse

Dat

arkliui

to horse

Acc

arkli

horse1

Ins

arkliu

with horse

Loc

arklyje

at horse2

Voc

arkly

horse!

 

1 The difference of Nom and Acc is "I" and "me".

2 Or "on", or even "in".

 

Table 1: One of the 5 Lithuanian nominal declensions

 

            The above Lithuanian example shows that the last 1-2 syllables of the noun changes according to the case (here: -ys, -io, -iui, -i, -iu, -yje, -y). This is declension. Generally different declensional schemes coexist; in Lithuanian five, with further subclasses.

I must confess that I omitted an "accent mark" in Acc. Hittite had one more case, the Ablative, sometimes kept in Latin, sometimes not. In Latin the system had been just degenerating when it has become frozen. It is in principle a 7-case system, but the Vocative differs from the Nominative only for 1 declensional class of the 5 (Brutus – Brute) and Locative is preserved only in topographic words (CarthagoCarthagini). Anyways, flectating languages do have good points. Obviously it gives a flexibility to a language if the different roles in the sentence are characterised by different forms. E.g. then the word order is more or less free, so it can be used for such purposes as emphasis, stress, &c. For English speakers this may be “complicated”, but look, modern Slavic languages preserve almost a Lithuanian-sized “complication”, even if in them the Vocative tends to coincide with the Nominative and Accusative sometimes either with Nominative or with Genitive. Every Slavic child learns a 6-case nominal paradigm in 3 years.

            Polysynthetic languages can mark "more than one" grammatical roles in one word, e.g. that the subject is singular or plural, and the object is singular or plural. This is "too much", at least to IE speakers.

            Agglutinative languages do not decline the noun according to cases, but put together unchanged roots and unchanged endings. (So they glue together blocks.) This is of course only the ideal situation; in living languages one can expect slight changes for easier pronunciation and such; but the rules are simple & unequivocal. So if the language borrows a word from somewhere, everybody can put that word to any grammatical role.

            To demonstrate what words look in an agglutinative language and how the system works, I take a Karaim example Karaim is an Eastern Kuman or Kipchak language, spoken in Lithuania & Crimea, and (besides of Hebrew letters) in Lithuania uses Latin letters (with more or less Lithuanian orthography). Turkish languages are rather strict in agglutinative rules. Karaim is near to the language of the Republic of Turkey, which the linguists call Ottoman Turkish, but that name is contrary to the will of Mustafa Kemal, the Atatürk. The Atatürk re-formed his nation against the traditions of the House of Ottoman/Osman founded in 13th century.Som, honouring the Atatürk, here I will use the formula “Ankara Turkish”.

         The expression: “for our doctors” is a single word (of course) in Karaim (as too in Ottoman Turkish & Magyar), but, contrary to Ottoman Turkish, lacks any international loanwords: savuchturuvčularymyzdan. It is built from blocks: sav-uch-tur-uv-ču-lar-ym-yz-dan. Sav- is the root, roughly “health”. (In Ottoman Turkish “sağ” = “right”.) The block –uch forms a verb, “get healthy” or “be healthy”. Then –tur makes the verb causative, (in Ottoman Turkish also –tur, more precisely –tur/-tür/-tır/-tir, a 4-form suffix, but that is Vowel Harmony, see Appendix),  so savuchtur is “to cure”.

         Then –uv (in Ottoman Turkish –u) forms nouns from verbs, and –ču (Ottoman Turkish –cu) is the person of the cure, so savuchturuvču = “doctor”. (And this is the reason that I started with Karaim, not the Ankara Turkish: in the latter savuchturuvču = hakım, from Arabic.

         Now, -lar makes Plurals, -ym marks the possession of First Persons, but the affixed –yz makes the possessor Plural; and finally –dan is simply the Ablative ending (the last four endings are exactly the same in Ankara). So really sav-uch-tur-uv-ču-lar-ym-yz-dan = “from our doctors”.

         In Magyar it is “orv-os-a-i-nk-tól”, slightly shorter. A good Magyar example (albeit not so overwhelming as the Karaim one, is a word we tell when drinking (so cca. “Cheers!): Egészségedre! So “To thy health. Here “Egész” is “whole”. The suffix “-ség” makes an abstract noun, so “egészség” is “health”. The suffix “-d” marks the Sg2 possessor, and “-re” is “to, onto”.

         My guess is that Turkish languages have the strictest rules of agglutination. But for quantity maybe Manyshi has the greatest number of agglutinative verbal endings in the Old World, and I think that Magyar is the first for the number of nominal endings.

         People generally believe that agglutinative languages operate with postposition, while flectating ones apply prepositions. While this is true in many cases, I do not think we should adhere to this idea. Anyways, Latin has a few postpositions (e.g.: Exempli gratia), and Oscan likes postpositions [18]. However it seems to be easier to show the cases and other relations at the end.

            In present lots of agglutinative languages are known. All Uralic languages are agglutinative. At the Eastern end of the Old World Japanese is agglutinative and on the Western end Basque.

            The grammatical role of a noun can be shown by the ending ("case") or by independent words: prepositions in English or postpositions in Magyar. Japanese uses exclusively postpositions, but there is no great difference between an unchanged postposition following separately the noun and unchanged endings glued to the root unchanged. In Magyar the difference is clearer; but that will be Chap. 3 and the Appendix.

            Different agglutinative languages have different numbers of cases. As I told, that is 1 for Japanese. It is 5 (or 6) for Manyshi, nearest relative of Magyar, and cca. 12 for Finnish. That is complicated, ne ce pas?

 

3. THE MAGYAR AGGLUTINATIVE CASES

            My first language is Magyar, the majority language of Hungary. As far as I know, this language has the most nominal cases in the Old World. No problem in using the full system for anybody: a few cases are archaic, so maybe not everybody uses every cases; but this is a minor problem and if somebody forgot a case, he can correct this in cca. 3 trials.

            There is no definite statement in the literature about the exact number of Magyar cases. However the number is cca. 30. I have found a relative old university textbook [19] trying to list them, and I start with this. However anybody can come up with 1-2 more cases, including me.

            Following the Finnish examples, Magyar cases have Latin names. But outside of Hungary nobody uses these names (lot of the cases being absent even in Finnish) and in Hungary maybe a handful. So I give the abbreviations, but maybe you would confuse an Illative and a Lative.

            English equivalents are given by prepositions. For Acc there is no preposition (English prepositions go with Acc, so "on me", not "on I"), and sometimes the preposition would not be unique.

            Being Magyar a living language, gluing together does imply minor changes. They are of 3 types:

            1) If the ending starts with a -v-, then this -v- changes into the end-consonant of the root, if there is an end-consonant.

            2) If the root ends with a vowel, that vowel may lengthen; the rules are almost unequivocal, and if not, both ways are good.

            3) If the root ends with a consonant and the ending is a consonant, then sometimes a vowel is put in between; the rules are not always unequivocal, but often lax and never important except style.

            Magyar has a Vowel Harmony Rule, as Turkish and Finnish do. So some endings have 1 form, some 2, some 3. The system is unequivocal, and will be shown in the Appendix. Here I give the forms for words of back vowels, especially for doboz=box. The definite article ("the") is "a" before consonant and "az" before vowel, but I omit the article.

            Direction Triality is a general IE idea: for pronouns English has: where - whither - whence. Here we start with nouns, but the idea of direction triality is easiest to demonstrate with these 3 pronouns in English. In Magyar adjectives and numerals do not differ for cases from nouns; pronouns are slightly more complicated. In Magyar Nominative is the Absolute Case: no ending.

 


Num

Case

Suffix

Suffix classes

Example

English

 1

Nom

-

-

doboz

box

 2

Acc

-t

1

dobozt1

box

 3

Gen1

-nak a2

2

doboznak a

of box

 4

Gen2

3

1

dobozé

of box

 5

Dat

-nak4

2

doboznak

to box

 6

Loc1

-n

1

dobozon

on box5

 7

Subl

-ra

2

dobozra

onto box6

 8

Delat

-ról

2

dobozról

from box

 9

Iness

-ban

2

dobozban

in box

10

Illat

-ba

2

dobozba

into box

11

Elat

-ból

2

dobozból

from box

12

Adess

-nál

2

doboznál

at box

13

Allat

-hoz

3

doboizhoz

to box

14

Ablat

-tól

2

doboztól

from box

15

Fam1

-nott

3

(Doboznott)

at Boxes

16

Fam2

-ni

1’

(Dobozni)

to Boxes

17

Fam3

-nól

2

(Doboznól)

from Boxes

18

Term

-ig

1

dobozig

until box

19

Lat

-

2

dobozzá

turning into box

20

Loc2

-t(t)

1

dobozott

at/on box7

21

Instr

-val

2

dobozzal

with box

22

Comit

-stul

2

dobozostul

together with box

23

Distr

-nként

1

dobozonként

box by box

24

Num

-szor

3

dobozszor

as times as box8

25

Temp

-kor

1

dobozkor9

at box

26

Caus

-ért

1

dobozért

for box10

27

Quant

-nyi

1’

doboznyi

as much/many as box

28

Mod1

-ul

2

dobozul

as box

29

Mod2

-lag

2

dobozlag

as box11

30

Mod3

-st

1

dobozost

as box11

31

Mod4

-lan

2

dobozlan

as box11,12

32

Mod5

-int

1’

dobozint

as box13

33

Mod6

-ként

1

dobozként

as box

34

Mod7

-képpen

1

dobozképpen

as box

35

Delat2

-ól

2

dobozól

from box14


 

 

 1 "Direct object" (vs. subject, the Nominative).

 2 The language frequently avoids this case.

 3 This is formally another type of ending.

 4 The ending is the same in Basque. Perhaps the coincidence with IE is not accidental, but this is common Palaeolithic inheritance.

 5 The "indirect object" of English.

 6 The ending is the same in Basque!

 7 Rare.

 8 As many times as...

 9 Slightly forced with "box"; e.g. "when reaches the box".

10 Both cause and consequence; "why?"

11 Answer to "how?"

12 Archaic.

13 With this noun it is rather unnatural.

14 Fossile.

 

For Column 4 see Appendix.

 

Table 2: The Magyar nominal endings.

 

 

            Sure, a few on agglutinative "("case") endings are archaic, for which 31 & 35 are the best examples. 20 is also archaic, except for geographic names, postpositions and pronouns; it was the original Locative. I do not try to explain the differences of the modal cases 28-34 in English; in Magyar it is clear. 35 is fossile but it was/is the Old Delative and you find it in many postpositions even now.

            We have 4 (!) direction trialities. The fourth one can be used only with names (mainly family names), and only at some parts of the country. But otherwise...

            Of course, not all of these 35 nominal cases are "absolutely necessary for communication", but surely they do make the communication versatile. E.g. see Case 2, the Accusative. Lots of languages do not differentiate between Nom and Acc; and then the sentences "Peter beats Paul" and "Paul beats Peter" differ only in word order; so the word order cannot be used then to express something else, say emphasis. On the other way, proto-IE, older IE languages and some modern ones do distinguish; see Latin: "Petrus caedit Paulum"~"Paulum caedit Petrus"~"Petrus Paulum caedit" = “Peter beats Paul” versus "Paulus caedit Petrum" = “Paul beats Peter”. Similarly, you do not have to distinguish for form between Accusative and Dative; and in many cases even with wrong word order it is clear who gives whom to whom. However, imagine a simple Latin sentence about slave market with me, Aulus and Priscilla. The English scheme is: "I give A to B"="I give B A". Now, "Do Aulum Priscillae" is clearly not "Do Aulo Priscillam", while neither is impossible.

            The 4 different Directional Trialities clearly differ from each other. 6-8 is about the outer surface of "the box", and generally, but not always, the upper surface. 9-11 is about the inside, and 12-14 is about the immediate neighbourhood. While the fourth triality, 15-17 could be used only allegorically about a box, it is quite regular for a family with the last name Box. Then Doboznott means (I was) "at (members of) the family Box", Dobozni is (I went) "to (members of) the family Box" and Doboznól is (I come) "from (members of) family Box". True, Budapest Magyar do not use this 3 forms; instead there it is told: Dobozéknál/Dobozékhoz/Dobozéktól, an -ék- for "the group" and then Triality 12-14.

            Terminative 18 can be used either in space or in time; and it is not necessarily the final place/time. Either it is, so the motion went until Point T and has stopped, or we are still in motion but just now we have arrived at T so we moved just "until T". Lative 19 is "to change into" and such, in the sense of Mrs. Lot, who changed into a statue of salt, so she “sóbálvánnyá változott”, where „sóbálvány” is the salt statue and „változik” is „change into”. Second Locative 20 deserves a short Chapter of its own. Instrumental 21 and Comitative 22 do often coincide in 21 "when speaking loosely", as it does in English as well, but an instrument is clearly not a partner. Distributive 23 has nothing to do with Numeral 24: instead of "box" let us take "négy"="four". "Négyenként" is used if I give four something to each individual, or I admit always 4 persons through the door, while "négyszer" is used as "4 times..." either as a number or if a repeated act is repeated 4 times or such; and they have nothing to do with Quantitative 27, which is a quantity, "doboznyi pirula" is e.g. pills which roughly (could) fill a box. Temporal 25 is to express a time: e.g. "Négykor" is more formally "at 4 o'clock", "egykor"="in olden times", "alkonyatkor" is "at evening dusk" and so on. Causative 26 is Causative in Aristotelian sense, so both cause and consequence.

            The modal endings 28-34 are not always easy to translate into English without lengthy circumscriptions, and "box" is not the best example for modal finesses. But "angolul" is "in English" (speaking, writing, &c.). "Tevőleg részt venni" (where "tevő" is "doing" and "részt venni" is "to participate") is "to participate actively". "Örömest" is "gladly", where "öröm" is "gladness" ("örömest" is almost "örömmel", but not exactly). "Ideiglen" may sound archaic, but "ideiglenes" is commonly used as "temporary". Case 32 is rare, but "szerint" is a very frequent word meaning "according to"; "szer" is "implement". Cases 33 & 34 are almost synonyms, but only almost. "Kép" is "picture", and "-képpen" may mean that somebody acts "as if", or it may mean "in such a way". But "úriemberként veszteni" is "to lose as a gentleman", and here I could not write "úriemberképpen".

            Case 35 is Delat2. Its meaning is clearly a Delative and it occurs even if it is fossilized. I included it to understand structures of some Postpositions. Maybe Case 32 is dying; no new constructions are made. And Case 20 is special. But otherwise the system is alive and becomes richer and richer.

 

4. NA SLOVENSKE PO SLOVENSKU!

            Locatives of geographic places (e.g. villages, towns &c.) are sometimes special. E.g. in Russian, where Locative is preserved only as a case after some prepositions, Prepositions "v" and "na" can take Acc or Loc. With Acc they denote our Cases 10 & 7, "into" and "onto", Illative & Sublative, while with Locative case they are our cases 9 & 6, "in" and "on", Inessive & First Locative. However, a lot of Russian words, mainly geographic ones, have one Locative case after not "v" or "na", and another Locative case for "v" or "na". (Really then the first one is not a Locative case at all, and indeed, some Russian grammars call the case Prepositional, as if prepositions could not go with Acc, Gen, Dat and Instr as well in Russian.) E.g. take the Ukrainian peninsula Crimea; that is "Krym". Now, in "to speak about Crimea" you find the "common Locative" or “Prepositional”: "govorit’ o Kryme"; but if you tell about something which is in the peninsula, that is the "proper" Locative: "v Krymu". Also observe the situations in Latin when the Direction Triality can be expressed via pure nominal cases (no prepositions). One such construction is domi/domum/domo (in/to/from home), so (Recidive) Loc/Acc/Abl.

            Magyar has an interesting “anomaly”. Hungarian geographic names are generally in First Locative, Sublative or Delative when you mean "in, into, from", but places outside of Hungary go with Inessive, Illative or Elative. Before you would jump into political conclusions, look at the title of this Chapter.

            The title of this Chapter is a Slovakian chauvinist slogan: "(You should speak) Slovakian in Slovakia!". However I will not comment the content of the slogan but its form: the Loc1 ↔ Iness problem.

            Namely, in Slovakian "in Slovakia" is "na Slovenske", but "in Hungary" (rather: "in the Magyar lands", but let us pass) is "v Mad'arske". In Magyar mirror translation "na Slovenske" should be Loc1, but we translate it with Iness; and the same trick backwards. By other words, we Magyars use Loc1 with Magyar geographic names but Iness with Slovakian and outsider ones while Slovakians use the mirror translation of Magyar Loc1 (i.e. na + Loc) with Slovakian geographic names, but that of the Magyar Iness (so v + Loc) with Magyar and outsider ones! And so on.

            Now, the situation is even more complicated, but only with city names. Generally we use Loc1 with Magyar city names, not Inessive; but not always. Sopron is an important city just at the Austrian border, and that is its Magyar name: the German is Ödenburg and the Latin is Scarbantia. Then the rule would be: in Ödenburg = Sopronon. However it is "Sopronban", Inessive. Győr is a substantial western Hungarian city (again, I give the Magyar name; the German is Raab and the Latin is Arrabona), but in the "in" construction we cannot use Loc1, “in Raab” is definitely not "Győrön". One possibility is Iness: "Győrben", but there is Loc2 as well: "Győrött". Maybe the Inessive comes from the origin of "Győr"←"gyűrű=ring", from the Avar ringlike fortification ("Hring" in Early Medieval German); you are not supposed to be "on a ring" but "within". There is a substantial Szekler city (so Magyar-speaking) in Transylvania, called Csíkszereda. (The Roumanian name is Miercurea Ciuc.) Now, the name takes Inessive instead Locative, so Csíkszeredában, in Transylvania, but other Magyars do not know this and use the Locative.

There are no clear thumb rules for the exceptions. Most geographic names of the home country come in Loc1, but some in Inessive; and some can be also in Loc2 as well. If the Loc2 form is possible, then generally there is an alternative, either Loc1 or Iness, but never both.

            This Loc2 is old, not very active, and can act outside of geographic names as well. E.g. "helyütt-helyütt" is "here and there, but not everywhere" ("hely"="place"). "All together" is "együtt", where "egy" is "1", so "all in one place".

 

5. PRONOUNS

            Pronouns are sometimes more conservative than nouns. E.g. in English only some pronouns have Acc forms: I/me, thou/thee, he/him, she/her, they/them and who/whom. Let us see now the cases of personal and demonstrative pronouns. "Én" is the Sg1 "I", and "ami, mi" is "what". I give only the first 11 cases for economy. The demonstrative pronoun is rather "ami" in statements and "mi" in questions.

 

Num

Case

Sg1

Dem.

English

 1

Nom

én

mi

I, what

 2

Acc

engem

mit

me, what

 3

Gen1

nekem a

minek a

of me/what

 4

Gen2

enyém

mié

my (mine), of what

 5

Dat

nekem

minek

to me/what

 6

Loc1

rajtam

min

on me/what

 7

Subl

rám

mire

onto me/what

 8

Delat

rólam

miről

from me/what

 9

Iness

bennem

miben

in me/what

10

Illat

belém

mibe

into me/what

11

Elat

belőlem

miből

from me/what

 

 

Table 3: Some cases of 2 pronouns.

 

 

Now, you can see that  the demonstrative pronoun exactly follows the nominal paradigm. But personal pronouns differ. In Accusative Sg1 and Sg2 does not take the -t ending, but expresses Acc otherwise; and also in Gen2; and in the other Cases something very similar to the nominal endings but somewhat longer come first, and then am -m to express Sg1 (and -d for Sg2). The end-m/d is a regular feature in Uralic languages to express possession, and you will understand everything after the next Chapter.

            Now, there are demonstrative pronouns for spatial position, with primitive Direction Triality. Let us see mirror translations for an interrogative one:

 

Where

hol

Whither

ho

Whence

honnan

 

Table 4: The primitive Pronominal Direction Triality.

 

OK, the second is the Lative, but you cannot recognise the endings in the first and third. They are old endings forgotten in the case paradigm. The system has been rearranged in the next 2 millenia.

            Now see a Demonstrative Direction Triality:

 

Here

itt

Hither

ide

Hence

innen

 

Table 5: The primitive Directional Triality of “here”.

 

            We see the Loc2 in the first line, a somewhat changed Lat in the second, and again the unnamed/unnumbered case in the third. One feels that it is a Delative, but neither 8, the New, nor 35, the Old. OK, pronouns can be more fossilized than nouns.

 

6. POSTPOSITIONS

            The case system contains only 35 cases, so cannot be sufficiently versatile. To help it, there are postpositions. E.g. we have seen that a "motion" in connection of the surface of a box is expressed via First Locative, Sublative or Delative, according to the Direction Triality. This is generally enough if the upper surface of the box is meant. But if the lower surface is in context, then we take the old word "*al > alj = bottom", and put proper (albeit old) endings according to the Triality, and these upgraded words we put behind the word for box. If my explanation is not clear enough, the Table may help. There is an alternative: a postposition or a possessive structure with Case endings on the possessed. And the two will not be equivalent for meaning, although close enough.

 

Postposition construct

Possessive construct

English

doboz alatt

doboz alján

under box

doboz alá

doboz aljára

to below box

doboz alól

doboz aljáról

from under box

 

Table 6: A postpositional Directional Triality

 

In the first line you see the proper Loc2 of the old word *al>alj=bottom in Column 1. This is used if the place is under the bottom of the box. However if the place is exactly the lower surface of the box, then it is "on the bottom of the box". The "bottom of box" is "doboz alja", a correct possessive construct with modern words in it, and then we append it with simply Case 6, the Loc1. When there are proper cases, one uses the cases. Still, anybody can see the direct connection between "doboz alatt" and "doboz alján" even if the first one uses Loc2, not Loc1.

            The situation is similar in Row 2. Al->alá (Lat) ~ aljra (Sublat); surely Lative preceeded Sublative in the same way as Second Locative preceeded First Locative. (The extra -á-in "aljára" is for the possessive expression: "onto its bottom".) In the third row "alól" is an older variant of "aljról"; surely the -ól acted 2 millenia ago and it is the Old Delative. (You may get the impression that –ról < -ra + -ól, so Delat < Loc1 + Old Delat.) This is not bad as first guess, even if the story was somewhat more complicated.)

            A very big variety of postpositions exist and I doubt if anybody has counted them. "Fölött/fölé/fölül(ről)" is the same Direction Triality with "föl" instead of "al": "föl" is a quite standard word, now for the uppermost layer of boiled milk, so these postpositions are used if we are upward from the box. (In this work I ignore verbs. But I note that fel-/föl- is also a verbal prefix meaning "up".) If we are behind, we take "meg/mög". Now the word denotes the completion of acts, but several centuries ago it meant the farther side, and a motion is  more or less completed if you have gone behind the barrier. So: "mögött/mögé/mögül": {Loc2/Lat/Delat2} of “mög”. If the place is in front of the box, then you take "*el". The front part of something is the "eleje" of that something, so this is again the archaic form of the word, and then: "előtt/elé/elől". If the place is besides the box, then we take the word "mell". That now means "breast", but maybe two millenia ago it meant "shoulder" too. For any case, if somebody stands besides me, then we stand shoulder to shoulder. So the right & left sides of the box define places on the two sides "mellett/mellé/mellől". Unfortunately there is no postposition (yet) distinguishing "right side/left side".

            Now a brief excursion to history/genealogy. Our next linguistic relatives are the Manyshi & Khanty in Westernmost Siberia, so we can check the reconstructions on Manyshi. Most of our cases are postpositions there, so maybe they were such in Old Magyar too (later). Our "mögött/mögé/mögül" is "sist/sisigh/sisnöl" in Manyshi, and "sis" is "backside". Now for the 3 endings, "igh" is homologous with our Lative ending (- Case 19), -t is just our Second Locative (Case 20) and -nöl is our Third Familiar Direction (-nól, Case 17), and the end of it is homologous to our "-ól", 35 Delat2, the Old Delative. So "mögött/mögé/mögül" is homologous to "sist/sisigh/sisnöl", only the roots differ.

            In some Manyshi postpositions the Magyar Lative (Case 19) is substituted with the regular modern Manyshi Lative. (I know the sentence looks strange. Evolution is strange.) The modern Manyshi Direction Triality endings are: Loc/Lat/Abl: -t/-n/-nöl. (So their Locative is our Second Locative, their New Locative is our First Locative and their Ablative is our Old Delative or rather the Third Familiar.) Now an interesting Manyshi Triality of Postpositions is the Modern and Quite Regular "kiwört/kivörn/kiwörnöl", with the same meaning as our 3 case endings Inessive/Illative/Elative "-ban/-ba/-ból". No surprise: the root is "kiwör"="the inside".

            Now it is time to return to the personal pronouns. We have seen some cases of "I"; now I give 6 cases of "I" and "thou":

 

Num

Case

Sg1

Sg2

Sg1

Sg2

 1

Nom

én

te

I

thou

 2

Acc

engem

téged

me

thee

 5

Dat

nekem

neked

to me

to thee

 9

Iness

bennem

benned

in me

in thee

10

Illat

belém

beléd

into me

into thee

11

Elat

belőlem

belőled

from me

from thee

 

Table 7: Some pronominal cases (of personal pronouns).

 

Now, the character code here is as follows. Bold is the Sg1/2 possessive ending, while Italics is an ending triality of some old Case endings -n/-(v/-ől which are Loc1/Lat/Old Delat. Now, what remained (except a vowel -e- which is an old story and we can tell that it is for easier pronunciation even if this is not true)?

            We see that Acc comes from Nom (and one more, Gen2, and no more). The Dative of Magyar is a mystery up to now; but surely there was an old word meaning a goal of a motion. This (unknown) word was longer, but we can tell for the moment that it was cca. "neke"~"being goal". (In Manyshi our Case 5 is a Postposition, "nopöl", but its root is unknown.) Then "neke-m" is "my neke/being goal" and "neke-d" is "thy neke/being goal". I do not believe that people reading this site are specially interested in unsolved details of the origin of Magyar Dative, so origin of "*neke" may remain temporarily hazy.

            But the situation is much clearer for Cases 9-11. There we see an old word, "*bele". Surely, the precursor of Iness/Illat/Elat was a Direction Triality of Postpositions. (The assumed l>n change in Inessive might come from assimilation to the Locative -n.) But what was the original root of the postposition?

            In Modern Magyar "bél" is "guts", but some centuries ago it meant generally "insides". We had 4 kings called "Béla", and of course that name did not come either from dirty guts or from the feminine Slovakian adjective "white". But the insides are priceless, well defended &c.

            Then the original postposition triality cca. "belen/bel/beleől" with the root "bele"="insides" is analogous with the Manyshi postpositional triality meaning our Cases 9-11 made from "kiwör"="insides".

            And now a check. The 3 endings appear in a prayer from 1195. Their forms then were "-ben/-bele/-belevl", but the writing did not mark length, and the probable reading is "-ben/-belé/-beleöl".

            This is exactly a Postposition Directional Triality formed from the word "inside", only it is already written together with the previous word. But in the prayer the ending does not yet follow the Vowel Harmony Rule ("milostben").

            Postpositions never take the vowel quality of the previous word. Well developed endings always do it (see Appendix). We do have records that some endings were still postpositions some centuries ago, and Case endings 25 & 34 now still violate the Vowel Harmony Rule. So our present Cases 9-11 were a fresh ending triality in 1195, and earlier were a postpositional triality as in Manyshi now.

            It is possible to repeat this reconstruction with two other Case Trialities (of course first/second personal pronouns cannot get the Familiar Triality). Let us see them in Sg1:

 

Num

Case

Magyar

English

 1

Nom

én

I

 6

Loc1

rajtam

on me

 7

Subl

m

onto me

 8

Delat

rólam

from me

12

Adess

nálam

at me

13

Allat

hozzám

to me

14

Ablat

tőlem

from me

 

Table 8: The Directional Trialities of nominal cases for a personal pronoun.

 

Again, I marked the possessive ending; what remains at the beginning, may be a postposition, changed in a millenium. Now, in Cases 6-8 the picture is clear: the original root was something "*ra(j)" (I think nobody is interested in tiny phonetic details) and then a Postposition triality ~"rajt/raj/rajól" was formed. In 1055 Case 7, the Sublative, is still a preposition, "rea". Its Loc member was unsuccessful to substitute the Loc1 -n, but the Cases of the personal pronouns are not the cases of a common root but mainly words with the same personal possessive ending.

            As for the Adessive/Allative/Ablative endings the above table shows that originally they were 3 independent prepositions. I think, you are not too interested, which ones.

 

7. CONCLUSIONS

            I think I demonstrated that with agglutination even a system of 35 cases + more than a hundred postpositions does not raise a problem. The rules are simple and the system is not arbitrary. If a dictator made the population happy but in turn he wanted to introduce 6 more Cases and 50 postpositions (partly for software operations, partly for family structure of the dictator), people would need a year to learn and then the bigger system would be as easy as the smaller one. If somebody introduces an ending of, say, "-éstar/éster" to refer to "transfer persons to the Second Reality”, then we can affix this to any noun, even if they really cannot be transferred to the Net.

            In Ref. [7] the Nantucketer expedition to Britain rescues a Princess of the Kurlelo line (planners of Stonehenge, astronomers &c.) whose first language is agglutinative and far relative to Tartessian, ancestor of Basque. The Nantucketers are overwhelmed by the agglutinative language, so learn Proto-Celtic and import a Tartessian captain to Nantucket. The result is a decade of wars.

            I think I demonstrated that with a Magyar in Nantucket (there are Latvians in the book, a half-Lithuanian and a Kuwaiti Arab) there would not have been a problem to learn Swindapa Kurlelo's language without the help of agglutinative Tartessians and then the story would have become much, much different.

            And there was another motivation behind this study as well. Remember, on All Saints' Day of Anno Domini 1612 in Florence there was a meeting in the house of the Archbishop. On this meeting a Dominican, Niccolň Lorini, made a statement that a certain Ipernicus had been in contradiction with the Scriptures. When later Galileo argued that the guy had been Copernicus, not Ipernicus, and his opinion was not in contradiction with the Scriptures, Lorini answered in a letter in a not too fanatic manner, writing that the Devil knows exactly what was the name of the Pole, and "I said a few words just that I should not seem dumb and to show that I also live" ([20]; my own English translation from the Magyar translation of the original Italian). Now, this work is a concise study about Magyar case endings for English speakers, also a comment to the Nantucket trilogy of Stirling; but also you may interpret it in the way of Lorini.

 

 

APPENDIX: VOWEL HARMONY AND NOMINAL ENDINGS

            The phenomenon called vowel harmony is present in the whole Turkic world and in Mongolian as well; the rules there are rather similar than in Magyar. In the Uralic group the vowel harmony system is full (but the system is slightly differs from the Turkish one) in Finnish and rudimentary in Estonian. So I cannot prove that vowel harmony was present in primordial Uralic, but it is rather probable because independent Turkic influence on Finnish and Magyar is rather far-fetched. The Rule is very useful for an agglutinative language, because it emphasizes the unity of the whole word produced from a root with many further elements glued together.

            The Rule in abstract sense (valid both for Uralic and for Altaic languages) is the following. The vowels are classified into groups A, B, ... The root may contain only vowels of one group. Let it be A. Now, the postpositions have alternatives PA, PB, ..., differing only in the vowel(s), and a root of type A takes postpositions of types PA; and so on.

            The Rule was never absolute. There are a few roots having mixed vowels. E.g. "apple" is "elma" in Ankara Turkish; both Magyar and Kazak correct it to the rule-abiding "alma". Compound words keep the "quality" of halves for long time. Verbal prefixes in Magyar do not participate in the Harmony. Some endings have not yet adapted to the Harmony; but the old records of Magyar do demonstrate that the process is going on.

            In Magyar the vowels are classified into 3 groups according to Vowel harmony. But first we must list the Magyar vowels; Magyar orthography is almost phonetic, but not exactly.

            First, Magyar vowels can be short vs. long. This characteristics is indifferent for vowel harmony. Length is indicated by prime(s) above the vowel according to the suggestion of Jan Hus, famous heretics and Bohemian nationalist leader at the beginning of XVth century, ending on the stakes according to the decision of the successful Synod of Contance, held under the auspice of the great King Sigismund I of Hungary & Croatia, also Western Emperor and pretender for the Bohemian throne. (Hus suggested such primes, hats, hooks &c. also for consonants but they were not accepted by us and Poles. Anyways, Hus was a heretic.)

            Counting the short/long variants too, there are 15 vowels, which may be Short/Long, Back/Front, Open/Mid/Closed and Rounded/Unrounded, using the English terminology. Magyar orthography (defined by the Hungarian Academy of Sciences) does not distinguish between two short "e"-sounds, Open and Mid, in some linguistic and musical publications written as e and ë, because the guy Kazinczy, having the biggest work in defining the new Magyar literary norm at the beginning of 1800's, lived at a territory where e and ë coincided. (This is just the birth area of Mihál Kováč, second President of the Independent Slovakia from 1993 too, so the ex-President speaks funny Slovakian as if he were a Budapest Magyar, what he is not.) I will use here the Magyar linguistic convention although it is not the best for foreigners.

            As for objective physical measurements, Magyar vowels are pronunciated clearly even in unstressed position so the acoustic analysis is not a problem; I refer to [21], [22] and citations therein.

            Table 9 shows the qualitative properties of the Magyar vowels:

 

Orthography

S/L

B/F

O/M/C

R/U

Closest familiar

A

S

B

O

R

Ĺ in Ĺngstrom

Á

L

B

O

U

Slovakian á

E

S

F

O

U

German ä

Ë

S

F

M

U

General e

É

L

F

M

U

Slovakian é

Ö

S

F

M

R

German ö

Ő

L

F

M

R

Long ö

O

S

B

M

R

General o

Ó

L

B

M

R

Long o

I

S

F

C

U

General i

Í

L

F

C

U

English ee

Ü

S

F

C

R

German ü

Ű

L

F

C

R

Long ü

U

S

B

C

R

General u

Ú

L

B

C

R

English oo

 

 

Table 9: The system of Magyar vowels

 

            Now, the quality of the root is classified as:

 

Root

Property

Back

B

FrontRounded

FR

FrontUnrounded

FU

Mixed

-

 

Table 10: The Magyar roots and the Vowel Harmony classes.

 

 

            The endings can belong to groups as follows:

 

Alternatives

As

In Table 2

3 endings

B/FR/FU

3

2 endings

B/F

2

1 ending

Indifferent

1

1 ending

Original B/F?

1'

 

Table 11: Vowel Harmony and Magyar case endings.

 

            Back roots generally take Back Case endings if there are alternatives. Front roots generally take front endings if there are alternatives; and for the trial endings they take the one proper for the last syllable of the root. Unique endings of course go with all roots.

            As for the vowels, let us see a list both for the root and for the endings (except for strictly unique endings where there are no alternatives):

 

Vowel

In root

In endings

A

B

B

Á

B

B

E

F/FU

F/FU

Ë

F/FU/-

F/FU

É

F/FU/-

F/FU

Ö

F/FR

F/FR

Ő

F/FR

F/FR

O

B

B

Ó

B

B

I

F/B

1'

Í

F/B

1'

Ü

F/FR

F/FR

Ű

F/FR

F/FR

U

B

B

Ú

B

B

 

Table 12: The system of Vowel Harmony.

 

            This Table is not complicated at all. For the seeming complications, ë in roots is sometimes indifferent for B/F opposition; é is the common long pair of both e and ë; and i/í are inheritors of 2 back/front pairs (present in modern Ankara Turkish) so endings with a single i/í may be really a B/F pair. In roots the orthography (and maybe modern acoustics) does not show the Harmony Class, but even unisyllabic "i" roots belong to definite B and F classes.

            Now let us see one example from Table 2 for every possible class shown above:

 

Type

Case

Endings

Remark

3S

13 Allat

-hoz/-hëz/-höz

-

2L

08 Delat

-ról/-ről

@

2S

05 Dat

-nak/-nek

-

1'

27 Quant

-nyi/-nyi

$

1

25 Temp

-kor

$

 

@          Long ó/ő in endings evolved from two short vowels separated by some -gh- or such. So there were no original 3L endings, and so no present *-ról/-ről/-rél triality, even if [19] mentions a trial -tól/-től/-tél from XVIth century..

$           Type 1 endings are the latest case endings, evolved from individual postpositions (which never take the vowel quality of the noon), and the evolution is preserved in records. Ending 25, -kor, is the inheritor of the postposition koron = in that age/time, sometimes appearing even now. On the other hand, Endings 16, 27 (and maybe 18 & 32) may have been evolved from a B/F pair. Anyways, for roots even now szív (B) is a verb: suck, taking Back verbal endings (ignored in this work) while szív (F) is a noun: heart, taking the Front paradigm throughout. So szívom is "I suck it", but szívem is "my heart". No native speaker ever confuses the paradigms.

 

Table 13: Examples of alternatives in endings according to the Vowel Harmony Rule.

 

 

            Now a few words about the physics: elementary speech acoustics. Sounds of speech are oscillation of air, and the latter not being solid, simple longitudinal oscillations. So the only degree of freedom is some real-valued function of time F(t).

            Of course F(t) is directly measurable, but the fine structure of F(t) is very probably unimportant. For first step we produce F(t) as superposition of periodic terms (Fourier transform) as

              F(t) = ∫A(ν)eiνt                                                                                            (1)

ν being the frequency. Then

              f(ν) = A(ν)A*(ν)                                                                                             (2)

is the power spectrum.

            Now, for affricates, Scottish r-type sounds or clicks f(ν) will not be enough to characterize the sound of speech; but for stationary sounds will be (this is Ohm's Law), and, at least in familiar languages vowels or sibilants are such. The power spectra are easy to measure and for vowels they are smooth.

            Half a century ago Petersen & Barney demonstrated [23] that for American English the actual vowel can be identified with some 90% success by measuring the frequencies of the first two local maxima of f(ν); in the acoustic jargon we tell : "the locations of the first two formants", formants being the characteristics of the power spectrum f(ν), and a local maximum is a characteristic part without doubt. It seems that the statement about the importance of the first 2 formants are true for quite a few other languages as well. Further formants are either unimportant, or they carry some other information (intonation, local timbre &c.)

            Now we investigated this problem for Magyar vowels some 2 decades ago [21], [22] (and you may find the citations therein useful); and here I recapitulate only one result.

            Tables 9-12 gave the system of Magyar vowels for formation and for the Vowel Harmony Rules. Now let us see the first two formant frequencies; all short vowels except ë (which is difficult to measure in Budapest) and á & é (which is the long counterpart of ë). (The other long ones seem rather coincide with their short pairs.) For male speakers and for population averages we get:

 

Vowel

1st formant, Hz

2nd formant, Hz

Á

757±10

1309±12

A

554±8

986±14

E

529±9

1713±21

Ë

381±3

2162±33

Ö

398±6

1516±22

O

410±5

908±20

U

292±7

811±10

Ü

277±6

1812±28

I

263±6

2262±30

 

Table 14: Magyar male formant frequencies.

 

            A pattern emerges. One ultra-open vowel, á; then 2 open ones, a & e, with equal first formant frequencies, but for the second ones higher for the front vowel. Then in the Mid degree 3 vowels, ë, ö & o, with the same first formant frequencies but with higher and higher second ones as B<FR<FU. Finally, at the Closed degree 3 vowels, i, ü & u, the lowest first formant frequencies (but again practically equal) and again the sequence B<FR<FU for the second formant frequencies. (We were looking for two i's, front and back, but were not yet successful.)

            We have the female frequencies as well but maybe that would be too much. For any case, the first formant frequencies are practically identical with the male ones; the seconds are of course significantly higher.

            So Magyar children learn in early childhood from the mothers to produce identical first formant frequencies for a & e, for o, ö & ë, and for u, ü & i. Also, boys and girls learn to form identical first formant frequencies for the respective vowels in spite of the different sizes of the speech channel; for the second formants they may fulfil the gender roles. Why? No such thing in American English.

            Who knows? But this is surely a property of vowel recognition in the Magyar linguistic community. And look: this is rather similar to Tables 11 & 13. An ending may not have variants. But if it has, it may be 2, but then it is generally a/e or u/ü, or may be 3, but then it is always o/ö/ë. So for the Magyar ears & brains the vowels with the same first formant frequencies are somewhat variants of each other: in the endings only the first formant frequency is given, the second is determined and unequivocally generated by the (second formant frequencies of the) vowels of the root. Is it not interesting for a non-agglutinative, for an IE speaker?

            The reader arrived at this point (if at all) maybe asks: this is very interesting but why to read just this? Now, the Appendix is given for two reasons.

            First, it demonstrates how automatic the system is, in spite of the multiplicity of endings because of Vowel Harmony. I mentioned in Chap. 7 that even if a future dictator would introduce by decree new words for his and only his blood relatives, people could use immediately the words in any grammatical context. Now, Table 12 of this Appendix shows how.

            Second, I told that the pronominal paradigm is slightly different from the nominal one; I gave examples in Chap 4 Table 3. Now, I show that for personal pronouns the paradigm is not only slightly different, but much wider: all postpositions are handled exactly as most Cases. Let us take the Sg1 personal pronouns in 5 Cases and with 3 Postpositions. The Cases will be Nominative, Accusative, and one of the 4 direction Trialities: Adessive, Allative and Ablative. Now, Postpositions are more versatile than Cases and are used if the Cases do not offer fine enough distinction: e.g. the {Adess, Allat, Ablat} Direction Triality tells us only that the position (static, end or start) is near to the referred point. If you want to tell where near, namely in front of, behind, above, below or besides, you must turn to the Postpositions. If the position is near, beside, then you use a Postpositional Triality {mellett, mellé, mellől}. So: doboz mellett = beside box. But if something is beside X, then it is at X, so doboznál, but not everything at the box is beside the box. However, without doubt, the Postpositional triality {mellett, mellé, mellől} is close kin to {Adess, Allat, Ablat} for meaning. So this Postpositional Direction Triality will represent the Postpositional "Cases" of the Sg1 personal pronoun. The small part of the comparison of the nominal and pronominal paradigms goes as follows:

 

Nom

doboz

box

én

I

Acc

dobozt

box

engem

me

Adess

doboznál

at box

nálam

at me

Allat

dobozhoz

to box

hozm

to me

Ablat

doboztól

from box

tőlem

from me

PP1

doboz mellett

beside box

mellettem

beside me

PP2

doboz mellé

to beside box

mellém

beside me

PP3

doboz mellől

from beside box

mellőlem

beside me

 

Table 15: Two Directional Trialities for a personal pronoun.

 

 

Here italics stands for the ending/postposition, while boldface is the Sg1 "possessive" ending. As I told in Chap. 5, in most cases of the personal pronouns "the endings come first and the person is represented by the possessive ending; and in Chap. 6 I told that most Cases evolved from postpositions, which in Magyar could, of course, be appended by possessive pronouns.

            However in (the overwhelming majority of) the Nominal Cases the Vowel Harmony works, so now the Cases are indeed Cases, meaning that they are shown by endings of integral words. But if the 35 cases of Table 2 form the nominal paradigm, then all the constructions similar to those e.g. in Table 14 belong to the (personal) pronominal one. But then the latter is longer than 100 rows; I think nobody ever counted them.

            Now, it is not always unequivocal to reconstruct the original postpositions which have degenerated to case endings. However the cases of the personal pronouns still keep the original class in Vowel Harmony, e.g.

 

in = -ban/ben, but

 

in me

bennem

in thee

benned

in him

benne

 

Table 16: The Inessives of the Magyar singular personal pronouns.

 

so surely the original postposition was Front, Unrounded and probably Open. Indeed, as already told, old records suggest a form bel(e), whose close kin is bél = guts, and belső = inside part.

            I told that the origin of the Adessive, Allative, Ablative Triality endings is not so clear. But, anyways, the paradigm of the personal pronouns shows that the origin of the dual Adessive ending -nál/-nél was Back (Open, and probably originally Unrounded), most probably some *nagha; and that of the trial Allative -hoz/-hez/-höz must have been Back, Rounded, Mid, and surely Short. According to Berrár [19] the Khanty has a postposition khosya with exactly the same meaning as Magyar -hoz; the PFU *k+back vowel evolves quite regularly into kh+back in Khanty and Manyshi but into h+back in Magyar, and in the Khanty postposition the first vowel is indeed Back, Mid, Rounded and Short. As for the dual Ablative -tól/-től, the same method + educated guesswork would give Front Rounded, and the fact that there are not 3 representations of the Front suggests an original Close vowel and a second syllable; something *tüghe*tüwe + Delat2, the Old Delative. Now, *tüwe is near to the reconstructed old form of = the base of the shoot, root. The slight change of meaning probably went as: away from the root of sg → away from something, but the details are not yet clear.

 

 

REFERENCES

 [1]       H. Everett: Relative State Formulation of Quantum Mechanics. Rev. Mod. Phys. 29, 454 (1957)

 [2]       P. Anderson: Eutopia. In: H. Turtledove & M. H. Greenberg (eds.): The Best Alternate History Stories in the 20th Century. Del Rey, New York, 2001

 [3]       Poul Anderson: The House of Sorrows. In: All One Universe. TOR, New York, 1997

 [4]       S. M. Stirling: The Sky People. TOR, New York, 2007

 [5]       S. M. Stirling: In the Courts of the Crimson Kings. TOR. New York, in print, a few Chapters can be found on the Net

 [6]       E. Flint: 1632. Baen Books, New York, 2001

 [7]       S. M. Stirling: Island in the Sea of Time. Roc, New York, 1998

 [8]       S. M. Stirling: Against the Tide of Years. Roc, New York, 1999

 [9]       S. M. Stirling: On the Oceans of Eternity. Roc, New York, 2000

 [10]     S. M. Stirling: Blood Wolf. In: H. Turtledove & Noreen Doyle (eds.): The First Heroes. TOR, New York, 2004

[11]      S. M. Stirling: Dies the Fire. Roc, New York, 2005

[12]      S. M. Stirling: The Protector's War. Roc, New York, 2006

[13]      S. M. Stirling: The Meeting at Corvallis. Roc, New York, 2006

[14]      S. M. Stirling: The Sunrise Lands. Roc, New York, 2007

[15]      S. M. Stirling: The Scourge of God. In preparation, a few Chapters can be found on the Net

[16]      Aristotle of Stageira: Opera omnia. Bekker N° 858a14-17

[17]      E. Shapir: Language: An Introduction to the Study of Speech. Harcourt, Brace & Co., New York, 1921 (especially Chapter 6)

[18]      C. D. Buck: A Grammar of Oscan and Umbrian. Evolution Publ., Bristol, 2005

[19]      Jolán Berrár: Magyar történeti mondattan. Tankönyvkiadó, Budapest, 1957

[20]      A. Favaro & al.: Le opere di Galileo Galilei; edizione nazionale sotto gli auspicii di sua maesta il re d'Italia. Barbera, Florence, 1890-1909; Vol. XI, p. 427

[21]      I. Borbély & B. Lukács: The Deviation Functional of Phoneme Recognition. Acustica 63, 129 (1987)

[22]      I. Borbély & B. Lukács: Symmetries in the Phoneme Recognition. Acustica 68, 52 (1989)

[23]      G. E. Petersen & H. L. Barney: Control Methods Used in a Study Of the Vowels. J. Acoust. Soc. Amer. 24, 175 (1952)